In which style of attachment do infants tend to turn away from the mother when she returns to the room?

  • Journal List
  • Paediatr Child Health
  • v.9(8); 2004 Oct
  • PMC2724160

Paediatr Child Health. 2004 Oct; 9(8): 541–545.

Language: English | French

Abstract

Attachment theory is one of the most popular and empirically grounded theories relating to parenting. The purpose of the present article is to review some pertinent aspects of attachment theory and findings from attachment research. Attachment is one specific aspect of the relationship between a child and a parent with its purpose being to make a child safe, secure and protected. Attachment is distinguished from other aspects of parenting, such as disciplining, entertaining and teaching. Common misconceptions about what attachment is and what it is not are discussed. The distinction between attachment and bonding is provided. The recognized method to assess infant-parent attachment, the Strange Situation procedure, is described. In addition, a description is provided for the four major types of infant-parent attachment, ie, secure, insecure-avoidant, insecure-resistant and insecure-disorganized. The antecedents and consequences of each of the four types of infant-parent attachment are discussed. A special emphasis is placed on the description of disorganized attachment because of its association with significant emotional and behavioural problems, and poor social and emotional outcomes in high-risk groups and in the majority of children who have disorganized attachment with their primary caregiver. Practical applications of attachment theory and research are presented.

Keywords: Attachment, Attachment relationships, Infant-parent attachment

Résumé

La théorie de l’attachement est l’une des théories les plus populaires et les plus empiriques à être reliée au rôle parental. Le présent article vise à examiner certains aspects pertinents de la théorie de l’attachement et certaines observations tirées des recherches sur l’attachement. L’attachement est un aspect précis de la relation entre un enfant et un parent, dont l’objectif consiste à ce que l’enfant se sente en sécurité, sécurisé et protégé. L’attachement est différencié d’autres aspects du rôle parental, tels que la discipline, l’amusement et l’enseignement. Des idées fausses courantes sur ce qu’est l’attachement et ce qu’il n’est pas sont abordées. La distinction entre l’attachement et les liens affectifs est présentée. La méthode de la « situation étrange », reconnue pour évaluer l’attachement entre le nourrisson et le parent, est décrite. Les quatre principaux modèles d’attachement entre le nourrisson et le parent sont également décrits, soit les modèles sécure, anxieux-ambivalant, anxieux-évitant et désorganisé-désorienté. Les antécédents et les conséquences de chacun de ces quatre modèles d’attachement entre le nourrisson et le parent sont étudiés. L’attachement désorganisé est décrit de manière plus approfondie, en raison de son association avec des troubles affectifs et comportementaux marqués, avec des issues sociales et affectives négatives dans les groupes très vulnérables et chez la majorité des enfants qui présentent un attachement désorganisé avec la principale personne qui s’occupe d’eux. Des applications pratiques de la théorie de l’attachement et des recherches sont présentées.

Parents play many different roles in the lives of their children, including teacher, playmate, disciplinarian, caregiver and attachment figure. Of all these roles, their role as an attachment figure is one of the most important in predicting the child’s later social and emotional outcome (1–3).

DEFINITION

Attachment is one specific and circumscribed aspect of the relationship between a child and caregiver that is involved with making the child safe, secure and protected (4). The purpose of attachment is not to play with or entertain the child (this would be the role of the parent as a playmate), feed the child (this would be the role of the parent as a caregiver), set limits for the child (this would be the role of the parent as a disciplinarian) or teach the child new skills (this would be the role of the parent as a teacher). Attachment is where the child uses the primary caregiver as a secure base from which to explore and, when necessary, as a haven of safety and a source of comfort (5).

Attachment is not ‘bonding’. ‘Bonding’ was a concept developed by Klaus and Kennell (6) who implied that parent-child ‘bonding’ depended on skin-to-skin contact during an early critical period. This concept of ‘bonding’ was proven to be erroneous and to have nothing to do with attachment. Unfortunately, many professionals and nonprofessionals continue to use the terms ‘attachment’ and ‘bonding’ interchangeably. When asked what ‘secure attachment’ looks like, many professionals and nonprofessionals describe a ‘picture’ of a contented six-month-old infant being breastfed by their mother who is in a contented mood; they also often erroneously imply that breastfeeding per se promotes secure attachment. Others picture ‘secure attachment’ between a nine-year-old boy and his father as the father and son throw a ball in the backyard, go on a fishing trip or engage in some other activity. Unfortunately, these ‘pictures’ have little, if anything, to do with attachment, they are involved with other parental roles (eg, their role as a caregiver in the case of the breastfeeding mother and as a playmate in the case of the father and son playing catch in the backyard). One might ask why the distinction between attachment and ‘bonding’ matters. The answer may lie in the fact that ‘bonding’ has not been shown to predict any aspect of child outcome, whereas attachment is a powerful predictor of a child’s later social and emotional outcome.

TYPES OF ATTACHMENT AND THEIR ANTECEDENTS

There are four types of infant-parent attachment: three ‘organized’ types (secure, avoidant and resistant) and one ‘disorganized’ type (Table 1). The quality of attachment that an infant develops with a specific caregiver is largely determined by the caregiver’s response to the infant when the infant’s attachment system is ‘activated’ (eg, when the infant’s feelings of safety and security are threatened, such as when he/she is ill, physically hurt or emotionally upset; particularly, frightened). Beginning at approximately six months of age, infants come to anticipate specific caregivers’ responses to their distress and shape their own behaviours accordingly (eg, developing strategies for dealing with distress when in the presence of that caregiver) based on daily interactions with their specific caregivers (7–9). Three major patterns of responses to distress have been identified in infants, which lead to three specific ‘organized’ attachment patterns.

TABLE 1

Types of attachment and antecedents

Quality of caregivingStrategy to deal with distressType of attachment
Sensitive Loving Organized Secure
Insensitive Rejecting Organized Insecure-avoidant
Insensitive Inconsistent Organized Insecure-resistant
Atypical Atypical Disorganized Insecure-disorganized

Infants whose caregivers consistently respond to distress in sensitive or ‘loving’ ways, such as picking the infant up promptly and reassuring the infant, feel secure in their knowledge that they can freely express negative emotion which will elicit comforting from the caregiver (9). Their strategy for dealing with distress is ‘organized’ and ‘secure’. They seek proximity to and maintain contact with the caregiver until they feel safe. The strategy is said to be ‘organized’ because the child ‘knows’ exactly what to do with a sensitively responsive caregiver, ie, approach the caregiver when distressed. Infants whose caregivers consistently respond to distress in insensitive or ‘rejecting’ ways, such as ignoring, ridiculing or becoming annoyed, develop a strategy for dealing with distress that is also ‘organized’, in that they avoid their caregiver when distressed and minimize displays of negative emotion in the presence of the caregiver (9). The strategy is said to be ‘organized’ because the child ‘knows’ exactly what to do with a rejecting caregiver, ie, to avoid the caregiver in times of need. This avoidant strategy is also ‘insecure’ because it increases the risk for developing adjustment problems. Infants whose caregivers respond in inconsistent, unpredictable and/or ‘involving’ ways, such as expecting the infant to worry about the caregiver’s own needs or by amplifying the infant’s distress and being overwhelmed, also use an ‘organized’ strategy for dealing with distress; they display extreme negative emotion to draw the attention of their inconsistently responsive caregiver. The strategy is said to be ‘organized’ because the child ‘knows’ exactly what to do with an inconsistently responsive caregiver, ie, exaggerate displays of distress and angry, resistant responses, ‘hoping’ that the marked distress response cannot possibly be missed by the inconsistently responsive caregiver. However, this resistant strategy is also ‘insecure’ because it is associated with an increase in the risk for developing social and emotional maladjustment.

Approximately 15% of infants in low psychosocial risk and as many as 82% of those in high-risk situations do not use any of the three organized strategies for dealing with stress and negative emotion (9). These children have disorganized attachment. One recently identified pathway to children’s disorganized attachment includes children’s exposure to specific forms of distorted parenting and unusual caregiver behaviours that are ‘atypical’ (10,11). Atypical caregiver behaviours, also referred to as “frightening, frightened, dissociated, sexualized or otherwise atypical” (10), are aberrant behaviours displayed by caregivers during interactions with their children that are not limited to when the child is distressed. There is evidence to suggest that caregivers who display atypical behaviours often have a history of unresolved mourning or unresolved emotional, physical or sexual trauma, or are otherwise traumatized (eg, post-traumatic stress disorder or the traumatized victim of domestic violence) (12).

MEASUREMENT

The three ‘organized’ strategies (secure, avoidant and resistant) are assessed in the Strange Situation (SS) (7), a 20 min laboratory procedure where patterns of infant behaviour toward the caregiver following two brief separations are categorized as secure or insecure (avoidant or resistant). The SS can be used when infants are 12 to 20 months old. Infants with secure attachment greet and/or approach the caregiver and may maintain contact but are able to return to play, which occurs in 55% of the general population (9). Infants with insecure/avoidant attachment fail to greet and/or approach, appear oblivious to their caregiver’s return and remain focused on toys, essentially avoiding the caregiver, which occurs in 23% of the general population (9). Infants with insecure/resistant attachment are extremely distressed by the separations and cannot be soothed at reunions, essentially displaying much distress and angry resistance to interactions with the caregiver, which occurs in 8% of the general population (9).

As with the ‘organized’ strategies, disorganization is measured using the SS, and the Main and Solomon’s (13,14) scoring scheme for disorganization. When distressed, infants who used a disorganized strategy for dealing with distress display unusual or disorganized behaviours in the SS, including misdirected or stereotypical behaviour, simultaneous display of contradictory behaviours, stilling and freezing for substantial periods, and direct apprehension or even fear of the parent. Such behaviours are particularly meaningful when they are intense and occur in the presence of the parent (9,14). They reflect an inability of the infant with disorganized attachment to find a solution to fear and distress, so the infants (momentarily) display bizarre or contradictory behaviour. Infants with disorganized attachment face an unsolvable dilemma: their haven of safety is also the source of their fear and distress (9). When infants face this dilemma, the three ‘organized’ strategies are not efficient in restoring feelings of safety and security in the presence of the attachment figure (13,15).

OUTCOME

Longitudinal research has shown that having a ‘loving’ primary caregiver and developing ‘organized and secure’ attachment to a primary caregiver acts as a protective factor against social and emotional maladjustment for infants and children (16,17). Attachment insecurity (avoidant and resistant) has been proven to be a risk factor for later development, but its high base rate in the normal population (approximately 40%) has reduced its predictive value for psychopathology (2).

Of the four patterns of attachment (secure, avoidant, resistant and disorganized), disorganized attachment in infancy and early childhood is recognized as a powerful predictor for serious psychopathology and maladjustment in children (2,18–24). Children with disorganized attachment are more vulnerable to stress (25,26), have problems with regulation and control of negative emotions (9), display oppositional, hostile, aggressive behaviours and coercive styles of interaction (20,27–31). Disorganized attachment is over-represented in groups of children with clinical problems and those who are victims of maltreatment (eg, nearly 80% of maltreated infants have disorganized attachment) (32–34). The combination of disorganization and a parental rating of a difficult temperament is a potent predictor of aggressive behaviour in children at five years of age (35). In addition, disorganized attachment in infancy has been linked to internalizing and externalizing problems in the early school years (20,36), poor peer interactions and unusual or bizarre behaviour in the classroom (37), and higher teacher ratings of dissociative behaviour and internalizing symptoms in middle childhood (19). Concurrent disorganized/controlling behaviour rated in the preschool and early school years related to oppositional defiant disorders in boys (38), parent-rated externalizing and internalizing problems (30), and high levels of teacher-rated social and behavioural difficulties in class (39,40). Children classified as disorganized with their primary caregiver at ages five to seven years have lower mathematics attainment at eight years of age (39). These academic problems appear to be mediated through effects on self-esteem and confidence in the academic setting (2). Children with disorganized attachment have low self-esteem (41), and at nine years of age are more often rejected by peers (42,43).

Adolescents who had disorganized attachment with their primary caregiver during infancy have higher levels of overall psychopathology at 17 years of age (19), and those classified as disorganized at five to seven years of age exhibit impaired formal operational skills and self-regulation at 17 years of age (44). Finally, children with disorganized attachment are vulnerable to altered states of mind, such as dissociation in young adulthood (19,45). A meta-analysis of 12 studies (n=734) addressing the association of disorganization and externalizing behaviour problems (9), found effect sizes ranging from 0.54 to 0.17, with a mean correlation coefficient of 0.29. The presence of negative findings suggests that the relation is not straightforward: Lyons-Ruth (28) found that 25% of children with disorganized attachment in infancy were not disturbed at seven years of age. Nonetheless, it appears that the majority of children with disorganized attachment suffer adverse outcomes.

SUMMARY AND PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS

A discussion of intervention in situations where there are difficulties in the infant-parent attachment relationship is beyond the scope of the present article; however,

  • The quality of the infant-parent attachment is a powerful predictor of a child’s later social and emotional outcome.

  • By definition, a normally developing child will develop an attachment relationship with any caregiver who provides regular physical and/or emotional care, regardless of the quality of that care. In fact, children develop attachment relationships even with the most neglectful and abusive caregiver. Therefore, the question is never, ‘is there an attachment between this parent and this child?’ Instead, the question is, ‘what is the quality of the attachment between this parent and this child?’

  • Children develop a hierarchy of attachments with their various caregivers. For example, a child with three different caregivers (mother, father and nanny) will have a specific attachment relationship with each caregiver based on how that specific caregiver responds to the child in times when the child is physically hurt, ill or emotionally upset; particularly, when frightened. If the mother reacts in loving ways most of the time, the child will develop an organized and secure attachment with the mother. That same child could develop an organized, insecure and avoidant attachment with the father if the father reacts in rejecting ways to the child’s distress most of the time. That same child could develop a disorganized attachment with the nanny if the nanny displays atypical behaviours during interactions with the child and has unresolved mourning or trauma.

  • In situations with multiple foster placements, neglect or institutionalization, children may develop disorders of nonattachment (49).

  • Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) is a special problem. The diagnosis of RAD, whether using criteria from the International Classification of Diseases: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines (46) or Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (47), was developed without the benefit of data, and research evidence to support its validity are still sparse (2). Zeanah and his colleagues (48,49) criticized the criteria for RAD as inadequate to describe children who have seriously disturbed attachment relationships rather than no attachment relationships. Another significant problem with the psychiatric diagnosis of RAD is that it suggests that the attachment difficulties lie within the child (ie, it is the child who receives the psychiatric diagnosis), when in fact, attachment involves the relationship between a child and caregiver. Finally, to my knowledge, there is no convincing empirical evidence to suggest that RAD is associated with any of the four types of attachment (secure, avoidant, resistant and disorganized).

  • Will letting an infant cry during the first six months of life affect the attachment relationship between that infant and the caregiver who lets the infant cry? Many child protection workers and health and mental professionals recommend that parents place a child safely in a crib when frustrated or angry instead of shaking the baby. Such a recommendation should continue to be made; however, one should closely monitor how frequently the parent needs to place the child in the crib and not respond. It is also acceptable for a child to cry when intrusive medical procedures need to be done to save the life of a child, treat a sick infant or give immunizations. Although, it may be advisable to have the primary caregiver present and promptly hold and comfort the infant. However, letting a baby cry because it is ‘good for their lung development’ (as some parents argue clinically), because it will ‘spoil’ the baby or because the baby needs to find their own ways to self-soothe might not be advisable during the first six months of life. Similarly, it is acceptable to let a baby cry during the second six months of life when the crying is not related to attachment (eg, when the child is not physically hurt, ill or frightened/emotionally distressed). Therefore, it is acceptable, from an attachment perspective, to use the Ferber method (50) or another sleep method, but only if the child does not have an ear infection, teething, etc.

  • During the first six months of life, promptly picking up a baby who is crying is associated with four major outcomes by the end of the first year of life. First, the baby cries less. Second, the baby has learned to self-soothe. Third, if the baby needs the caregiver to soothe him/her, the baby will respond more promptly. And finally, the caregiver who responded promptly and warmly most of the time (not all the time; nobody can respond ideally all of the time) to the baby’s cries, will have created secure, organized attachment with all of the associated benefits.

REFERENCES

1. Dozier M, Stovall KC, Albus KA. Attachment and psychopathology in adulthood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 497–519. [Google Scholar]

2. Green J, Goldwyn R. Annotation: attachment disorganisation and psychopathology: new findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:835–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Greenberg MT. Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 469–98. [Google Scholar]

4. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss. Volume 1: Attachment. 2nd edn. New York: Basic Books; 1982. [Google Scholar]

5. Waters E, Cummings EM. A secure base from which to explore close relationships. Child Dev. 2000;71:164–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Klaus MH, Kennell JH. Maternal-Infant Bonding: The Impact of Early Separation or Loss on Family Development. St Louis: Mosby; 1976. [Google Scholar]

7. Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MD, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]

8. Sroufe LA. The role of infant-caregiver attachments in development. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T, editors. Clinical Implications of Attachment. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1988. pp. 18–38. [Google Scholar]

9. van IJzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants and sequelae. Dev Psychopathol. 1999;11:225–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Lyons-Ruth K, Bronfman E, Atwood G. A relational diathesis model of hostile-helpless states of mind: Expressions in mother-infant interactions. In: Solomon J, George C, editors. Attachment Disorganization. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 33–70. [Google Scholar]

11. Schuengel C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Blom M. Unresolved loss and infant disorganization: Links to frightening maternal behavior. In: Solomon J, George C, editors. Attachment Disorganization. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 71–94. [Google Scholar]

12. Zeanah CH, Danis B, Hirshberg L, Benoit D, Miller D, Heller SS. Disorganized attachment associated with partner violence: A research note. Infant Ment Health J. 1999;20:77–86. [Google Scholar]

13. Main M, Solomon J. Discovery of an insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In: Brazelton TB, Yogman MW, editors. Affective Development in Infancy. Norwood: Ablex; 1986. pp. 95–124. [Google Scholar]

14. Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg MT, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the Preschool Years. Chicago: University Press; 1990. pp. 121–60. [Google Scholar]

15. Main M, Hesse E. Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening behavior the linking mechanism? In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the Preschool Years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 161–82. [Google Scholar]

16. Egeland B, Hiester M. The long-term consequences of infant day-care and mother-infant attachment. Child Dev. 1995;66:474–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. van IJzendoorn MH, Sagi A, Lambermon MWE. The multiple caretaker paradox: Data from Holland and Israel. In: Pianta RC, editor. New Directions for Child Development No 57 Beyond the Parent: The role of Other Adults in Children’s Lives. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1992. pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]

18. Boris NW, Fueyo M, Zeanah CH. Clinical assessment of attachment in children under five. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:291–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Carlson EA. A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganized/disoriented. Child Dev. 1998;69:1107–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Lyons-Ruth K, Block D. The disturbed caregiving system: Relations among childhood trauma, maternal caregiving and infant affect and attachment. Infant Ment Health J. 1996;17:257–75. [Google Scholar]

21. Lyons-Ruth K, Alpern L, Repacholi B. Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Dev. 1993;64:572–85. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Main M, Cassidy J. Categories of response to reunion with the caregiver at age six: Predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a one-month period. Dev Psychopathol. 1988;24:415–26. [Google Scholar]

23. Main M, Morgan H. Disorganization and disorientation in infant strange situation behavior: Phenotypic resemblance to dissociative states? In: Michelson LK, Ray WJ, editors. Handbook of Dissociation: Theoretical, Empirical and Clinical Perspectives. New York: Plenum Press; 1996. pp. 107–38. [Google Scholar]

24. Zeanah CH, Boris NW, Larrieu JA. Infant development and developmental risk: A review of the past 10 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:165–78. Erratum in: 1998;37:240. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Hertsgaard L, Gunnar M, Erickson MF, Nachmias M. Adrenocortical responses to the strange situation in infants with disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships. Child Dev. 1995;66:1100–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Splanger G, Grossmann KE. Biobehavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Dev. 1993;64:1439–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Greenberg MT, Speltz ML, DeKlyen M. The role of attachment in the early development of disruptive behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol. 1993;3:413–30. [Google Scholar]

28. Lyons-Ruth K. Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64:64–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Lyons-Ruth K, Easterbrooks MA, Cibelli CD. Infant attachment strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7. Dev Psychol. 1997;33:681–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Solomon J, George C, De Jong A. Children classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and at school. Dev Psychopathol. 1995;7:447–63. [Google Scholar]

31. Speltz ML, Greenberg MT, Deklyen M. Attachment in preschoolers with disruptive behavior: A comparison of clinic-referred and nonproblem children. Dev Psychopathol. 1990;2:31–46. [Google Scholar]

32. Cicchetti D, Barnett D. Attachment organization in maltreated pre-schoolers. Dev Psychopathol. 1991;3:397–411. [Google Scholar]

33. Lyons-Ruth K, Connell D, Zoll D, Stahl J. Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Dev Psychol. 1987;23:223–32. [Google Scholar]

34. Lyons-Ruth K, Repacholi B, McLeod S, Silva E. Disorganized attachment behavior in infancy: Short-term stability, maternal and infant correlates, and risk-related subtypes. Dev Psychopathol. 1991;3:377–96. [Google Scholar]

35. Shaw DS, Vondra JI. Infant attachment security and maternal predictors of early behavior problems: A longitudinal study of low-income families. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995;23:335–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Lyons-Ruth K, Jacobvitz D. Attachment disorganization: Unresolved loss, relational violence and lapses in behavioral and attentional strategies. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of Attachment. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 520–54. [Google Scholar]

37. Jacobvitz D, Hazan N. Developmental pathways from infant disorganization to childhood peer relationships. In: Solomon J, George C, editors. Attachment Disorganization. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]

38. Greenberg MT, Speltz ML, Deklyen M, Endriga MC. Attachment security in pre-schoolers with and without externalizing problems: A replication. Dev Psychopathol. 1991;3:413–30. [Google Scholar]

39. Moss E, Rousseau D, Parent S, St-Laurent D, Saintonge J. Correlates of attachment at school age: Maternal reported stress, mother-child interaction, and behavior problems. Child Dev. 1998;69:1390–405. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Goldwyn R, Stanley C, Smith V, Green JM. The Manchester Child Attachment Story Task: Relationship with parental AAI, SAT and child behavior. Attach Hum Dev. 2000;2:71–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Cassidy J. Child-mother attachment and the self in six-year-olds. Child Dev. 1988;59:121–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Verschueren K, Marcoen A. Representation of self and socioemotional competence in kindergartners: Differential and combined effects of attachment to mother and to father. Child Dev. 1999;70:183–201. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Verschueren K. Narratives in attachment research. Biennial meeting of the European Society of Developmental Psychology; Uppsala. 2001. [Google Scholar]

44. Jacobsen T, Edelstein W, Hofmann V. A longitudinal study of the relation between representations of attachment in childhood and cognitive functioning in childhood and adolescence. Dev Psychol. 1994;30:112–24. [Google Scholar]

45. Hesse E, van IJzendoorn MH. Parental loss of close family members and propensities towards absorption in offspring. Dev Sci. 1998;1:299–305. [Google Scholar]

46. World Health Organization . International Classification of Diseases: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. 10th edn. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]

47. American Psychiatric Association Diagnositic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edn(DSM-IV)Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 1994 [Google Scholar]

48. Zeanah CH, Mammen OK, Lieberman AF. Disorders of Attachment Handbook of Infant Mental Health. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. pp. 332–49. [Google Scholar]

49. Zeanah CH, Boris NW. Disturbances and Disorders of Attachment in Early Childhood Handbook of Infant Mental Health. 2nd edn. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. pp. 358–81. [Google Scholar]

50. Ferber R. Solve Your Child’s Sleep Problems. New York: Simon & Schuster; 1985. [Google Scholar]


Articles from Paediatrics & Child Health are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press


Which attachment pattern involves an infant who continues playing when his or her mother leaves the room and ignores her when she returns?

A child with an avoidant attachment style will avoid or ignore the mother, showing little emotion when the mother departs or returns. The child may run away from the mother when she approaches.

When a baby acts upset because a caregiver is leaving?

When a baby acts upset because a caregiver is leaving, the baby is exhibiting: separation anxiety.

What best describes an insecurely attached child?

It avoids the mother. What best describes an insecurely attached child? It involves mastering intellectual skills. It occurs from about the age of 6 to puberty.

What type of attachment is Ben likely to show?

Ben is likely to show insecure-resistant attachment.