Reading and listening comprehension Pdf

Talk in the Classroom.

  • B. Power
  • Linguistics, Education

  • 2001

Traditionally, language has been divided into four main skills – listening, speaking, reading and writing. Until the mid-19 century, schools tended to focus on the written language. Foreign languages

  • View 1 excerpt, cites methods

Reading Psychology, 26:55–80, 2005

Copyright C

2005 Taylor & Francis Inc.

0270-2711/05 $12.00 + .00

DOI: 10.1080/02702710590910584

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LISTENING AND READING

COMPREHENSION OF DIFFERENT TYPES OF TEXT AT

INCREASING GRADE LEVELS

IRENE-ANNA N. DIAKIDOY

Department of Psychology, University of Cyprus, Nicosia, Cyprus

POLYXENI STYLIANOU

Agrokipia Elementary School and University of Cyprus, Nicosia, Cyprus

CHRISTINA KAREFILLIDOU and PANAYIOTA PAPAGEORGIOU

Department of Psychology, University of Cyprus, Nicosia, Cyprus

This study examined the hypotheses that (a) the relationship between listening

and reading comprehension becomes stronger after decoding mastery; (b) the dif-

ference between listening and reading decreases with increasing grade level; and

(c) similar patterns of relationship and difference are obtained with narrative

and expository texts. The sample included 612 students in Grades 2, 4, 6, and

8. Students read and listened to two narratives and two expository texts and

completed corresponding comprehension tests that were in the form of sentence

verification tasks. The findings confirmed the first two hypotheses but not the

third one. In the case of expository text, the relationship between listening and

reading comprehension was weaker than the corresponding one with narrative

text, and performance levels were comparable across all elementary grades. More-

over, reading comprehension levels were higher than listening comprehension levels

in Grade 8, regardless of text type. The implications of these findings with respect

to the dominant unitary process model and the assessment and instruction of oral

and written language comprehension are discussed.

By the time children enter school, they have acquired to various

degrees the skills that enable them to use and understand oral lan-

guage in a variety of contexts (Snow, 1983; Wells, 1986). Despite

the acknowledged differences between oral and written language

This research was supported by an Elva Knight Research Grant from the International

Reading Association to the first author.

We thank George Spanoudes for his help in data analyses, and the students for their

enthusiastic participation and their teachers and principals who made their participation

possible.

Address correspondence to Irene-Anna N. Diakidoy, Department of Psychology, Uni-

versity of Cyprus, P.O. Box 20537, Nicosia CY-1678, Cyprus. E-mail: eddiak@ucy.ac.cy

55

56 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

(Perfetti, 1987; Tannen, 1985), such skills have been commonly

associated with later reading achievement. This assumption un-

derlies much of the emergent literacy research (Eller, Pappas, &

Brown, 1988; Feitelson, Goldstein, Iraqi, & Share, 1993; Senechal,

LeFevre, Thomas, & Daley, 1998) and the assessment of reading, as

indicated by the inclusion of listening comprehension measures in

various standardized tests and reading inventories (Burns & Roe,

1993; Johns, 1994; Kertoy & Goetz, 1995). Listening comprehen-

sion figures prominently as a component in theoretical models of

reading (e.g., Hoover & Gough, 1990; Joshi & Aaron, 2000) and

has been considered to provide the base for the acquisition of

reading comprehension skills (Sticht & James, 1984). The extent,

however, to which this is true across the reading tasks that differ-

ent text types represent has not received any attention. Therefore,

the present study examined the relationship between listening and

reading comprehension at increasing grade levels, and the extent

to which it is influenced by the type of text.

Differences between oral and written language concern both

the course of their development as well as the nature of the lin-

guistic stimulus they present. The acquisition of oral language pre-

cedes the acquisition of written language, and it is a natural process

that takes place gradually in highly contextualized situations. On

the other hand, the acquisition of written language depends on

formal, systematic instruction and occurs in more decontextual-

ized situations (Snow, 1983). Although spoken language is more

likely to contain fragments instead of complete sentences, listeners

can take advantage of extra-linguistic clues, such as gestures and

prosodic information, such as stress and intonation (Akinnaso,

1985; Sinatra, 1990). Moreover, listeners and speakers share the

same context, and, therefore, spoken language is characterized

by greater involvement and interaction (Akinnaso, 1985; Biber,

1982). Consideration of such differences has led to the advance-

ment of the dual process view which postulates that, although read-

ing and listening comprehension share some common elements,

they essentially represent different processes (Samuels, 1987; see

also Sinatra, 1990).

On the other hand, Sticht and his colleagues have advanced

the position that listening comprehension and reading compre-

hension are highly interrelated (Sticht, Beck, Hauke, Kleinman,

& James, 1974). Their extensive review of the available studies led

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 57

them to conclude that both listening and reading comprehen-

sion depend on the same general comprehension process. There-

fore, after decoding skills have been mastered, performance on

listening and reading comprehension tasks should be compara-

ble. Moreover, listening comprehension level represents a poten-

tial for reading comprehension. Specifically, they have argued that

listening comprehension skill facilitates the acquisition and pre-

dicts the level of skill that will be achieved in reading (Sticht et al.,

1974; Sticht & James, 1984). Their position essentially represents a

contrasting unitary process view, according to which the same com-

prehension process underlies both listening and reading (Sinatra,

1990).

Support for a unitary process view has come from studies that

have shown input modality (listening vs. reading tasks) to have no

effect on the recall of meaningful and syntactically well-structured

word strings (Guthrie & Tyler, 1976) and on the recall and sum-

marization of stories (Kintsch & Kozminsky, 1977; Smiley, Oakley,

Worthen, Campione, & Brown, 1977). Moreover, Sinatra (1990)

found a facilitative effect in comparison times when an identical

auditory stimulus preceded the presentation of two visual stim-

uli. Since that effect was manifested for meaningful sentences,

syntactic word strings, and random word strings but not for ran-

dom nonword strings, Sinatra (1990) concluded that listening and

reading processing converge at the lexical level as well. Additional

support for the unitary process view has also come from studies

that have shown directly listening comprehension to predict read-

ing comprehension skill (Curtis, 1980), and instructional inter-

ventions targeting listening comprehension to generally have pos-

itive effects on reading comprehension performance (Pearson &

Fielding, 1982; Sticht et al., 1974).

Although the relationship between listening and reading com-

prehension is assumed to be unidirectional early on, it is also ex-

pected that differences between listening and reading compre-

hension diminish over time and as a result of print exposure

(Perfetti, 1987; Sticht & James, 1984). On the basis of their re-

view, Sticht et al. (1974) concluded that up to Grade 7, listen-

ing comprehension ability is higher than reading comprehension

ability, whereas in adulthood the direction of the difference is re-

versed. Curtis (1980) found that the relationship between reading

skill and listening comprehension changed as a function of age,

58 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

with correlations at the Grade 2 level being significantly differ-

ent from the correlations at the Grade 5 level. More interestingly,

Hendrick and Cunningham (1995) found that wide reading was

associated with greater listening comprehension skill at Grade 4.

Although their study was not specifically designed to test direction-

ality, their results support the possibility of a reciprocal relationship

(Hendrick & Cunningham, 1995). The present study examined

the relationship and the differences between listening and read-

ing comprehension levels at Grades 2, 4, 6, and 8. On the basis

of previous research and claims, we expected the relationship to

become stronger and the differences to decrease with increasing

grade level.

The studies that examined the effects of presentation mode

on the recall and summarizing of text (i.e., Kintsch & Kozminsky,

1977; Smiley et al., 1977), as well as those that examined the effects

of listening to text being read on the acquisition of literacy and

learning in general (Eller et al., 1988; Feitelson et al., 1993), have

all used narrative texts. It can be argued, however, that printed nar-

rative text is closer to oral discourse with narrative characteristics,

such as contextual situatedness and temporal/causal sequences

(Biber, 1982; Graesser, Singer, & Trabasso, 1994), and which oc-

curs frequently in everyday conversation, such as when one nar-

rates what happened at work or over a weekend. Moreover, young

children can be typically expected to have had more exposure

to oral narrative text structures in highly interactive contexts that

guide and facilitate comprehension, such as story reading at home

(Neuman, 1996). Finally, the information in narrative text is more

likely to be familiar and its organization more likely to be pre-

dictable (Graesser, Swamer, Baggett, & Sell, 1996). Therefore, it

is not clear whether the strong relationship between listening and

reading comprehension that has been observed so far is also due

to mediating factors, such as the similarity of the communicative

task(s) that oral and printed narration represent, their high fre-

quency of occurrence in both modalities, and/or the familiarity

and concreteness of their content.

Kintsch and Kozminski (1977) and Sinatra (1990) have sug-

gested the possibility that listening and reading processes may

diverge when it comes to processing lengthy and difficult text.

Expository text is more likely to contain unfamiliar information

and to have an underlying abstract and logical structure (Singer,

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 59

Harkness, & Stewart, 1997). Moreover, expository text structures

are diverse (Englert & Hiebert, 1984), and, therefore, readers and

listeners cannot rely on the activation of a single schema the way

they can rely on the activation of a story schema to guide their com-

prehension of narrative text. Although students are increasingly

expected to read and learn from expository text with increasing

grade level, they have little opportunity to encounter and inter-

act with this text type in the early grades (Duke, 2000). Finally,

oral expository discourse, such as lectures and public speeches, is

much more infrequent than oral narrative discourse. It can be ar-

gued, then, that these factors—unfamiliar content and structure

and low frequency of occurrence—increase the relative difficulty

of expository text in terms of processing demands and in com-

parison to narrative text. Therefore, in the present study both ex-

pository and narrative texts were presented orally and in printed

form.

In summary, the purpose of this study was to evaluate (a) the

relationship between listening and reading comprehension at in-

creasing grade levels, and (b) the effects of text type on listening

and reading comprehension levels. With respect to this second

goal, it was predicted that both listening and reading comprehen-

sion performance will be higher for narrative than expository text.

However, it was also expected that with increasing grade level the

differences between listening and reading comprehension of nar-

rative and expository text will decrease as a result of increased

exposure to oral and written expository structures. Finally, of inter-

est was also the extent to which the relationship between listening

and reading comprehension evolves in a similar manner across the

different text types, as would be predicted by the unitary-process

view.

Method

Participants

The sample included 612 students enrolled in three elementary

schools and two middle schools located in different medium-

sized towns in the island nation of Cyprus. Overall, there were

135 second graders (65 males and 70 females), 151 fourth graders

(79 males and 72 females), 151 sixth graders (70 males and

60 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

81 females), and 177 eighth graders (95 males and 82 females).

All students were fluent speakers of Greek. In order to obtain

an indication of Receptive Language Ability across grade levels,

the method developed by Levin et al. (1997) was adapted and

employed in this study. Specifically, the names of the students in

each participating classroom were printed on separate index cards.

Then each classroom teacher was asked to evaluate each student’s

written and oral language comprehension ability by sorting the

corresponding card into one of five categories: very high, high,

average, low, very low (Levin et al., 1997). In cases where teach-

ers classified the majority of their students into one or two cat-

egories only, they were asked to discuss at length any language

comprehension differences between the students within a cate-

gory and to reclassify accordingly. Teacher ratings ranged from

1to5with higher scores assigned to higher ability categories.

This measure indicated that the students, as a sample, represented

a wide range of Receptive Language Ability levels (M=3.15,

SD =1.23). In addition, their school achievement levels ranged

from low (3.5) to high (10.0) as indicated by previous year’s Grade

Point Averages (M=7.87, SD =1.50). The correlation coeffi-

cient between Teacher ratings and Grade Point Average was 0.77

(p=.00).

Materials

TEXTS

A preliminary list of forty short stories, articles, and excerpts

from books, magazines, and encyclopaedias was compiled. Seven

independent teachers, whose teaching experience ranged from

seven to 22 years, read and rated each text for topic familiarity,

number of potentially unfamiliar words, and overall text difficulty

for each grade level. Texts that were judged by all teachers to have

an unfamiliar topic but relatively few unfamiliar words, and to be

moderately difficult but appropriate for a given grade level were

included in the final list (see Appendix). From this list, two nar-

ratives (271 to 376 words long) and two expositories (262 to 356

words long) were selected for each grade. The expository texts ei-

ther described and elaborated on the characteristics of animals,

physical locations, archaeological findings, astronomical phenom-

ena, and groups of people (Appendix, Texts 1, 5, 8, 11, 13, and 16)

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 61

or presented facts and details related to socio-cultural topics and

issues (Appendix, Texts 6 and 14). Therefore, the predominant

expository structures were description and enumeration (Englert

& Hiebert, 1984). The selected texts were parsed into idea units

(van den Broek, 1989) by two independent graduate assistants. In-

terrater agreement ranged from 79% to 93%, and all differences

were resolved in conference. The selected narratives and exposi-

tories were comparable with respect to the number of idea units

per sentence (M=1.63, SD =.30, and M=1.77, SD =.36 re-

spectively, t(14) =−0.88, p=.39). Each selected text was printed

in a separate booklet and tape recorded.

COMPREHENSION TESTS

All comprehension tests were in the form of a sentence verifi-

cation task. For each text, a list of literal and inferential statements

was constructed. Literal statements represented single idea units

extracted verbatim from the text (Pearson & Johnson, 1978). In-

ferential statements represented implicit referential, causal, super-

ordinate goal, generalization, and comparison/contrast relation-

ships between idea units (Graesser et al., 1994). Two independent

raters evaluated each statement in each list as either literal or infer-

ential. Statements for which there was 100% agreement comprised

the preliminary comprehension tests whose length ranged from 17

to 22 statements.

Fifty-four undergraduate students enrolled in a cognitive psy-

chology course listened to each tape-recorded text and completed

the corresponding preliminary comprehension test. Items that

were not answered correctly by two or more students (18% of the

items, on the average, across tests) were excluded. From the re-

maining items, 14 were selected for inclusion in a final comprehen-

sion test for each text. Specifically, each final comprehension test

included seven literal and seven inferential statements, each fol-

lowed by a Yes/No option. In each test there was an equal number

of Yes and No correct responses. Each correct response received

a score of 1 and each incorrect response a score of 0. The scores

received by each student for each statement were summed and con-

verted to proportions, yielding six measures: Listening Compre-

hension, Reading Comprehension, Narrative Listening Compre-

hension, Expository Listening Comprehension, Narrative Reading

Comprehension, and Expository Reading Comprehension. The

62 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

order of statement presentation in each test was randomized, and

two comprehension test versions with different orders were created

for each text.

Procedure

For the purposes of the study, the students remained in their in-

tact classrooms, and data were collected during the first half of

the school year. The study was completed in two 40 min. sessions

scheduled from two to seven days apart. In each session, students

read and listened to one narrative and one expository text. As a

result, half of all the students in each grade level read a particular

text while the rest heard that same text. Presentation Mode (oral

vs. written), Text Type (narrative vs. expository), and order of pre-

sentation were counterbalanced between classrooms and sessions.

Students listened to or read each text only once. Subsequently, the

written text booklets were collected, and the students completed

the corresponging comprehension test. They were instructed to

read each statement carefully, to decide whether it was true on the

basis of the text they had just read or listened, and to circle Yes or

No accordingly. Comprehension test version was counterbalanced

between students.

Results

Relationship Between Listening and Reading Comprehension

Preliminary analyses indicated that all dependent variables,

Listening Comprehension, Reading Comprehension, Narrative

Listening Comprehension, Narrative Reading Comprehension,

Expository Listening Comprehension, and Expository Reading

Comprehension had normal distributions (skewness <1) and ho-

mogeneous variances across Grade levels (p>.05). Moreover,

one-way Analysis of Variance indicated that School had no signif-

icant effect, and, therefore, this variable was excluded from sub-

sequent analyses. Listening and Reading Comprehension Scores

were significantly correlated with each other at all grade levels ( p<

.01). However, this relationship became stronger with increasing

Grade level, and a series of Z-tests indicated that the correlation

coefficient obtained at Grade 2 (r=.44,p=.00) was significantly

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 63

TABLE 1 Mean Proportion Correct Responses to Listening and Reading

Comprehension Tests as a Function of Grade Level

Grade Listening Comprehension Reading comprehension

2(n=125) .65 (.14) .60 (.15)

4(n=132) .68 (.13) .69 (.13)

6(n=142) .75 (.14) .74 (.14)

8(n=163) .71 (.14) .75 (.14)

Note. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.

different from the coefficients obtained at all other Grade levels

(r=.63,p=.00,Z=2.14 to Z=2.25,p<.05).

Multivariate Analysis of Variance with Grade as the between-

subject factor, and Presentation Mode (oral vs. written) as the

within-subject factor, indicated there was a significant main effect

of Grade, Hotelling’s T2=.22, F(6, 1114) =19.92, p=.00, but

not of Presentation Mode, Hotelling’s T2=.00, F(1, 559) =0.06,

p=.81. There was, however, a significant Grade ×Presentation

Mode interaction, Hotelling’s T2=.07, F(3, 559) =13.60, p=.00.

It can be seen from Table 1 that Listening Comprehension is higher

in Grade 2, comparable in Grades 4 and 6, and lower in Grade 8

than Reading Comprehension. These differences are significant

at both Grade levels, paired t(124) =3.92, p=.00 at Grade 2 and

paired t(163) =−4.33, p=.00 at Grade 8.

It can also be seen from Table 1 that, whereas Reading

Comprehension increases with increasing Grade level, Listening

Comprehension shows a decrease at Grade 8. However, multiple

comparisons (Scheffe method) indicated that the Listening Com-

prehension performance of eighth-graders was not significantly

different from that of sixth-graders (p=.25, d=0.23). The dif-

ferences between second-graders and fourth-graders were also not

significant (p=.28, d=0.22). In contrast, the Listening Compre-

hension scores obtained by second-graders were significantly lower

than the scores obtained by both sixth-graders and eighth-graders

(p=.00, d=0.62). Finally, the Listening Comprehension perfor-

mance of fourth-graders was significantly lower than that of sixth-

graders (p=.00, d=0.50). With respect to Reading Comprehen-

sion, the performance of second-graders was significantly lower

from the performance of all other groups ( p=.00, d=0.95),

and the performance of fourth-graders was also significantly lower

64 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

from the performance of eighth-graders ( p=.00, d=0.43). In

contrast, the performance of sixth- and eighth-graders was com-

parable (p=.88, d=0.09), and the difference between the per-

formance of fourth- and sixth-graders approached significance

(p=.06, d=0.33).

Effects of Text Type

In order to examine the effects of Text Type and its interactions,

Multivariate Analysis of Variance was performed with Grade as the

between-subject factor, and Presentation Mode (oral vs. written)

and Text Type (narrative vs. expository) as the two within-subject

factors. In addition to the expected significant main effect of Grade

and its interaction with Presentation Mode ( p<.01), there was also

a significant effect of Text Type, Hotelling’s T2=.40, F(1, 559) =

225.23, p=.00, and a significant Grade ×Text Type interaction,

Hotelling’s T2=.11, F(3, 559) =19.48, p=.00.

It can be seen from Table 2 that, whereas Expository Compre-

hension increases steadily with increasing Grade level, Narrative

Comprehension decreases at Grade 8, with this decrease being

more pronounced when narrative text is presented orally. Over-

all, however, Expository Comprehension is significantly lower than

Narrative Comprehension (p<.05) except in Grade 2 and when

both texts are presented in written form, paired t(125) =0.48,

p=.63.

Multiple comparisons indicated that the Narrative Listening

scores of sixth-graders were higher than those of all the other

groups (p<.01, d=0.59). On the other hand, the Narrative

TABLE 2 Mean Proportion Correct Responses to Listening and Reading

Comprehension Tests as a Function of Grade Level and Text Type

Listening Reading

Grade Narrative Expository Narrative Expository

2(n=125) .69 (.17) .61 (.17) .60 (.19) .59 (.16)

4(n=132) .74 (.18) .62 (.15) .75 (.18) .64 (.16)

6(n=142) .82 (.17) .67 (.18) .80 (.17) .68 (.17)

8(n=163) .73 (.17) .70 (.15) .78 (.16) .73 (.16)

Overall .78 (.18) .65 (.17) .74 (.19) .67 (.17)

Note. Standard deviations are shown in parentheses.

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 65

Reading scores of the second-graders were lower than those of all

other groups (p<.01, d=1.08). With respect to Expository Lis-

tening Comprehension, the scores of the sixth- and eighth-graders

were higher than those of the second-graders (p=.01, d=0.47)

and the fourth-graders (p=.04, d=0.40). The Expository Read-

ing Comprehension scores of the second-graders were lower than

those of the sixth- and eighth-graders (p<.01, d=0.68), and

the scores of the fourth-graders were lower than the scores of the

eighth-graders only (p=.00, d=0.59). Otherwise, performances

were quite comparable.

Although the Presentation Mode ×Text Type interaction only

approached significance, Hotelling’s T2=.01, F(1, 559) =3.57,

p=.06, a series of paired t-tests were employed to allow a compar-

ison of difference patterns between Listening and Reading Com-

prehension across Text Types and within each Grade separately.

These analyses indicated that, overall, Narrative Listening Com-

prehension is higher than Expository Listening Comprehension,

paired t(566) =10.98, p=.00, and Narrative Reading Comprehen-

sion is higher than Expository Reading Comprehension, paired

t(564) =8.76, p=.00, (see also Table 2). Moreover, Narrative

Listening Comprehension is higher in Grade 2, paired t(126) =

4.41, p=.00, comparable in Grades 4 and 6 (p>.10), and lower

in Grade 8, paired t(163) =−3.81, p=.00, than Narrative Read-

ing Comprehension. In comparison, Expository Listening Com-

prehension is lower than Expository Reading Comprehension in

Grade 8, paired t(163) =−2.04, p=.04, and comparable at all

other Grade levels (p>.10).

Table 3 shows the correlation coefficients between Oral

and Written Narrative Comprehension and between Oral and

TABLE 3 Correlation Coefficients Between Listening and

Reading Comprehension as a Function of Grade Level and

Text Type

Grade Narrative text Expository text

2(n=127) .25.39

4(n=132) .50.15

6(n=143) .38.36

8(n=164) .49.34

p<.01.

66 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

Written Expository Comprehension. In the case of Narrative Text,

the relationship between Listening and Reading Comprehension

becomes stronger in Grade 4 and Grade 8. In fact, the correlation

coefficient obtained at Grade 2 is significantly different from the

correlation coefficients obtained at Grade 4 (Z=2.76, p<.05)

and at Grade 8 (Z=2.91, p<.05) only but not from the coefficient

obtained at Grade 6 (Z=1.79, p>.05). In contrast, in the case

of Expository Text, the correlation coefficient obtained at Grade

4isvery low and significantly different only from the correlation

coefficient obtained at Grade 2 (Z=2.58, p<.05).

A series of Regression analyses indicated that Narrative Listen-

ing Comprehension is the strongest predictor of Expository Read-

ing Comprehension in the higher Grades and the only significant

predictor in Grade 4 (see Table 4). In Grade 2, Narrative Reading

Comprehension accounts for the highest proportion of variance in

Expository Reading scores. In contrast, Expository Listening Com-

prehension is the weakest predictor and significant only in Grades

2 and 6 (Table 4). Moreover, it can be seen from Table 5, that Nar-

rative Listening and Reading Comprehension are, similarly, the

strongest or the only significant predictors of Expository Listening

Comprehension.

TABLE 4 Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Expository

Reading Comprehension Within Grade Levels

Variable βR2change Fchange p

Grade 2 (n=125)

Narrative Listening .25 .06 8.24 .00

Narrative Reading .36 .12 18.37 .00

Expository Listening .24 .04 6.85 .01

Grade 4 (n=132)

Narrative Listening .43 .18 29.24 .00

Narrative Reading .11 .01 1.56 .21

Expository Listening .05 .00 .27 .60

Grade 6 (n=142)

Narrative Listening .53 .29 56.25 .00

Narrative Reading .19 .03 6.56 .01

Expository Listening .19 .03 6.10 .01

Grade 8 (n=163)

Narrative Listening .45 .21 42.26 .00

Narrative Reading .22 .04 8.16 .00

Expository Listening .12 .01 2.42 .12

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 67

TABLE 5 Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Expository

Listening Comprehension Within Grade Levels

Variable βR2change Fchange p

Grade 2 (n=125)

Narrative Listening .44 .19 28.98 .00

Narrative Reading .30 .08 14.14 .00

Expository Reading .22 .04 6.85 .01

Grade 4 (n=132)

Narrative Listening .18 .03 4.41 .04

Narrative Reading .46 .16 25.24 .00

Expository Reading .05 .00 .28 .60

Grade 6 (n=142)

Narrative Listening .28 .08 11.88 .00

Narrative Reading .34 .10 16.24 .00

Expository Reading .23 .03 6.10 .01

Grade 8 (n=163)

Narrative Listening .42 .17 34.04 .00

Narrative Reading .36 .10 22.11 .00

Expository Reading .12 .01 2.42 .12

A parallel series of Regressions for Narrative Comprehension

indicated that Narrative Listening is the strongest predictor of Nar-

rative Reading Comprehension in Grades 4, 6, and 8, accounting

for 15–24% of its variance. In contrast, Expository Reading Com-

prehension accounts for the highest percentage of variance (12%)

in Narrative Reading scores in Grade 2. Similarly, whereas Narra-

tive Reading Comprehension is the strongest predictor of Narra-

tive Listening in the higher Grades, accounting for 15–25% of the

variance, Expository Listening accounts for the highest percentage

of variance (14%) in Narrative Listening in Grade 2.

Discussion

Listening versus Reading

In agreement with previous findings (Curtis, 1980; Sticht et al.,

1974), the present study indicates that the overall relationship be-

tween listening and reading comprehension becomes significantly

stronger after the second grade, when word decoding skills can be

assumed to have been mastered to a satisfactory degree. In addi-

tion, input modality is found to have no independent influence

68 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

on comprehension level. This finding is also consistent with previ-

ous findings showing no difference between listening and reading

conditions in the recall of sentences (Guthrie & Tyler, 1976) or

in the recall and summarization of stories (Kintsch & Kozminsky,

1977; Smiley et al., 1977). There was, however, a significant main

effect of grade level and this factor interacted with input modality.

As expected, both listening and reading comprehension increase

with increasing grade level. But listening comprehension shows a

significant improvement between Grades 4 and 6, whereas a cor-

responding significant improvement in reading comprehension

appears between Grades 2 and 4.

The timing of the increase in reading comprehension appears

to coincide with mastery of the initial decoding skills. On the other

hand, the timing of the increase in listening comprehension ap-

pears to relate to particular school practices. Specifically, the com-

prehension of oral language—including instructions, discussions,

and stories—is an explicit curriculum objective in the Cypriot ele-

mentary school (Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture, 1996).

This objective is achieved by having students recall, summarize,

and identify the main ideas or conclusions of what was discussed

by the group or read by the teacher. Moreover, the oral discourse to

which students are exposed and the associated instructional activ-

ities become notably more complex and extended in the fifth and

sixth grades. At these grade levels, students are also required to co-

ordinate and draw conclusions from discussions on social and liter-

ary issues as well as to interpret and critique orally-presented liter-

ary stories and poems (Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture,

1996).

The present findings also indicate that listening comprehen-

sion performance exceeds reading comprehension in the early

elementary-school grades. This difference disappears in the higher

elementary grades, and its direction is reversed in middle school,

where reading comprehension is found to be higher than listening

comprehension. These changes in the difference between listen-

ing and reading are found to occur earlier than previously sug-

gested (i.e., Sticht et al., 1974). These changes in conjunction with

the timing of the improvements observed in each modality suggest

that listening comprehension is also sensitive to instruction which,

if provided, results in listening and reading comprehension devel-

oping concurrently rather than independently (Royer, Sinatra, &

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 69

Schumer, 1990). The basic pattern, however, is consistent with the

hypothesis that, whereas listening is more efficient early on, the dif-

ference between listening and reading decreases with increasing

grade level (see also Sticht et al., 1974).

The finding that reading comprehension is comparable to lis-

tening in Grades 4 and 6 and higher than listening in Grade 8

can be attributed to a general increase in the emphasis on reading

and learning from text in the three higher elementary grades, as

indicated by the objectives stated for these grades (Cypriot Min-

istry of Education and Culture, 1996). Moreover, the transition to

independent reading and learning from text in the Cypriot mid-

dle school is a relatively abrupt one, since teachers at this level

are no longer expected to directly guide and support students’

comprehension of written text (Cypriot Ministry of Education and

Culture, 1993). The gradual increase in the emphasis on reading

and its eventual connection to learning and overall school achieve-

ment may have, in effect, promoted the development of more effi-

cient and effective skills and strategies for extracting information

from printed text (see also Sticht et al., 1974). In contrast, listen-

ing comprehension skills and strategies remain as an objective only

in connection with group discussions and specific literary genres,

such as poetry (Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture, 1993).

Narrative versus Expository Text

With respect to the type of text, expository comprehension level, in

general, is found to be lower than narrative comprehension level.

That is the case in all grades except in Grade 2, where the ability to

comprehend printed expository text is comparable to the ability to

comprehend printed narrative text. We consider this lack of differ-

ence early on to reflect the influence of less-than-perfect decoding

skills that can be expected to hinder the reading comprehension

of any text. It appears, however, that after decoding skills become

automatic, other factors, such as type of text, become important

and influence comprehension regardless of input modality.

Moreover, whereas expository comprehension increases

steadily after Grade 4, narrative comprehension decreases at Grade

8. This difference in developmental pattern appears, again, to re-

late to particular school practices that influence the frequency of

occurrence of and, therefore, students’ familiarity with each text

70 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

type at different grade levels. Although the staple of all reading and

listening in the Cypriot elementary school is the short story, the

gradual increase in the emphasis on learning from text necessitates

a corresponding increase in students’ exposure to informational

text after Grade 4 (Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture,

1996). At the middle school level, expository text dominates, with

the textbook being the primary medium of learning in all subjects

(Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture, 1993). In compari-

son, narrative text is used only in connection with the teaching of

ancient and modern Greek literature. Therefore, this decrease in

the comprehension of narrative text appears to reflect a decrease

in its frequency of occurrence and relative importance for overall

school achievement at the middle school level.

Although input modality was not found to interact signifi-

cantly with text type (Danks, 1980), the relationship and the dif-

ference patterns between listening and reading appear to diverge

to some extent depending on the type of text. For one thing,

listening to expository text is not more efficient than reading such

text at any grade level. Instead, the ability to comprehend exposi-

tory text develops similarly and concurrently in the two modalities

until reading becomes more efficient than listening in the eighth

grade. Therefore, any advantages associated with listening early on

appear to be confined to the case of narrative text only.

This finding contrasts with that of Royer, Kulhavy, Lee, and

Peterson (1986) who found that, with difficult text, fourth and

sixth graders’ listening comprehension was higher than their read-

ing comprehension. They also found the opposite to be true with

easy texts. Royer et al. (1986) used narratives that were classified

as easy or difficult depending on whether their readability level

was below or above their participants’ grade level. In comparison,

all texts used in the present study were judged to be appropriate

for our students’ level. Expository text, however, can be assumed

to represent a more difficult text type by virtue of its diverse struc-

tures, abstract organization, generally unfamiliar content, and rel-

atively low frequency of occurrence in elementary school (Duke,

2000; Englert & Hiebert, 1984; Singer et al., 1997). Nevertheless,

our students’ ability to comprehend these texts was the same re-

gardless of presentation mode. A listening comprehension advan-

tage was manifested only in connection with narrative text and

with the youngest students.

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 71

Furthermore, the relationship between listening and reading

evolves differently depending on the type of text. In the case of

narrative text, the relationship follows the expected pattern, be-

coming stronger after the second grade (Curtis, 1980; Sticht et al.,

1974). The opposite, however, appears to be true with expository

text. Correlation coefficients remain low and, except in Grade 2,

lower than those obtained with narrative text. Contrary to expec-

tations (i.e., Curtis, 1980), the relationship between expository lis-

tening and reading becomes non-significant in the fourth grade,

after decoding skills are in place, and remains modest in the higher

grades. In general, expository listening is not a strong or significant

predictor of expository reading across the grade levels examined.

In fact, expository listening and expository reading predict each

other to the same modest extent.

These findings might be attributable to a number of factors.

For one thing, oral expository text, as exemplified by extended

lectures and speeches, remains relatively rare in elementary and

middle school. In fact, the guidelines for elementary- and middle-

school teachers in the Cypriot National Curriculum (Cypriot Min-

istry of Education and Culture, 1993, 1996) advise teachers to avoid

lengthy presentations and to encourage, instead, group discussions

and hands-on activities. Such an approach, although undeniably

helpful and effective in many instructional situations, may con-

tribute to a general lack of familiarity with expository text struc-

tures and a specific lack of practice in comprehending such text

through listening. Therefore, the ability to comprehend oral ex-

pository text cannot be expected to predict the ability to compre-

hend written expository text. On the other hand, the increasing

exposure to written expository text may promote the development

of comprehension strategies that specifically take advantage of the

fact that readers have control over the rate they process text infor-

mation and the option to reprocess it as needed for coherence pur-

poses (i.e., Carlisle & Felbinger, 1991; Danks, 1980; Sinatra, 1990).

Expository text characteristics, such as unfamiliar content and di-

verse structures that accommodate abstract relationships (Englert

& Hiebert, 1984; Kintsch & Kozminsky, 1977; Singer et al., 1997),

may further necessitate a reliance on visual presentation factors

that help reduce the amount of information that needs to be kept

active in memory. Therefore, the development of presentation-

specific strategies in addition to general comprehension skills and

72 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

the notable absence of oral expository discourse may be responsi-

ble for the overall low relationships observed.

Prediction patterns are also found to vary across grade lev-

els. Whereas the ability to comprehend written narrative text is

the strongest predictor of expository reading comprehension in

Grade 2, the ability to comprehend orally-presented narratives be-

comes the strongest predictor in the higher grades and the only

predictor in Grade 4. Similarly, whereas the ability to compre-

hend written expository text is the strongest predictor of narrative

reading in Grade 2, the ability to comprehend orally-presented

narratives becomes again the strongest predictor in the higher

grades. These changes in prediction pattern appear to suggest that

(a) listening and reading processes are more different than sim-

ilar early on, with perceptual skills being the crucial factor and

regardless of text type (Perfetti, 1987); and (b) basic comprehen-

sion skills developed in the context of listening to narratives are

transferred and applied to some extent to the processing of writ-

ten text, again regardless of its type (Kintsch & Kozminsky, 1977;

Sticht & James, 1984). Moreover, the finding that narrative read-

ing comprehension level is the strongest predictor of expository

listening in Grades 4 and 6 appears to underscore the possibil-

ity that the comprehension of oral expository discourse may have

more in common with reading than listening (Akinnaso, 1985;

Biber, 1982; Danks, 1980). Nevertheless, the findings concerning

relationship and prediction patterns lend support to Hendrick

and Cunningham’s (1995) claim that the relationship between

listening and reading is more likely to be a reciprocal one than

unidirectional after decoding mastery.

Theoretical Implications and Limitations

Although the present findings lend some support to the domi-

nant unitary process perspective, they also suggest that its stronger

claims need to be qualified in some respects. First, the extent to

which listening exceeds reading early on (i.e., Sticht et al., 1974)

depends also on text factors in addition to any lingering decoding

difficulties. As a result, the claim that the development of listen-

ing comprehension ability precedes the development of reading

comprehension (Curtis, 1980; Kintsch & Kozminsky, 1977; Sticht &

James, 1984) cannot be generalized across discourse types. Second,

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 73

reversals in the direction of the difference cast doubt on the extent

to which listening comprehension represents an absolute potential

for reading comprehension (Sticht & James, 1984). Instead, they

suggest that listening comprehension skills, although probably

necessary early on, are not sufficient for becoming a skilled reader

after decoding mastery (see also Carlisle & Felbinger, 1991; Danks,

1980; Hendrick & Cunningham, 1995). Finally, the relationship be-

tween listening and reading cannot be taken for granted since it is

also mediated by text-related factors in addition to the perceptual

skill factors associated exclusively with reading (i.e., Curtis, 1980).

The present findings contribute to Samuels’ (1987) argument

that differences in the context, if not in modality, result in general

comprehension mechanisms functioning in somewhat different

ways. The type of the text represents one such difference that is

found to influence listening and reading comprehension levels

and their relationship in complex ways. Considering that the task

of the comprehender is to construct a mental representation of

what the text is about (McNamara, Miller, & Bransford, 1991),

there is no reason to suppose that the task itself and/or the ba-

sic processes by which it is accomplished varies as a function of

modality. Knowledge of text structure, however, can provide an or-

ganizational framework influencing, thereby, the coherence and

the elaboration of the resulting representation (i.e., Goldman &

Rakestraw, 2000). Conversely, lack of such knowledge may render

visual presentation factors important in the sense that a slower

reading pace and the ability to reprocess information can facili-

tate the identification of main ideas and connections. This is more

likely to be the case with expository text where lack of familiar-

ity with its diverse structures is compounded with unfamiliar and

more abstract content (Duke, 2000; Englert & Hiebert, 1984).

Although the above possibility can account for the weak re-

lationship between expository listening and reading, it cannot ex-

plain fully why expository listening and reading levels remain com-

parable across elementary grades. These comparable levels may

result from comprehension strengths and weaknesses specifically

associated with each modality. Carlisle and Felbinger (1991), who

also used expository text with fourth, sixth, and eighth graders,

found some students to be weak in listening comprehension only

while others to be weak in reading comprehension. Such differ-

ential patterns of ability may be responsible for the comparable

74 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

performances observed in this study. Moreover, given the empha-

sis on learning from expository text after Grade 4, any modality-

specific comprehension abilities may operate in parallel with a

more general academic ability with which reading ability, in par-

ticular, must be related. However, an examination of ability factors

was beyond the scope of the present study. Therefore, further re-

search is needed to examine the extent to which this is the case and

whether any ability influences are confined to expository text only.

Weak relationships and comparable levels are accompanied by

changes in the prediction patterns across grade levels and modal-

ity in the case of expository text. Whereas narrative listening skills

predict expository reading level, narrative reading skills predict

expository listening level after decoding mastery. These predic-

tion patterns appear to suggest (a) that narrative comprehension

skills provide the basis upon which additional or modified strate-

gies are developed to deal with different discourse types; and (b)

that narrative comprehension skills developed in each modality are

transferred and employed across modality in the particular case of

expository text. With respect to the first point, it can be argued that

narrative comprehension skills are developed earlier and practiced

extensively in the course of everyday conversation and story read-

ing at home. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that they will

support, at least to some extent, subsequent development in com-

prehension ability. The simultaneous influences across modality

and text type, however, were unexpected and remain puzzling.

Since we cannot preclude the possibility of this being due to the

materials used and/or to measurement error, further research is

necessary to establish whether this finding is replicable and, if yes,

to unravel the text- and, possibly, person-related factors that may

be responsible.

Further research is also necessary to establish the extent to

which the different results obtained with expository text are simply

due to lack of exposure and, therefore, familiarity with expository

structures. If that is the case, then comparable exposure to oral

expository and narrative text early on should give rise to similar

results concerning the relationship between listening and reading.

This, in turn, would provide additional evidence in support of the

hypothesis that the same general processes underlie both listen-

ing and reading comprehension. If, however, divergent patterns

of difference and relationship continue to be observed, then the

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 75

possibility of comprehension processes being fundamentally influ-

enced by the context and, possibly, by modality remains open.

Educational Implications

Although the general findings of the present study contribute to

the unitary process position, the particular pattern of results ap-

pear to support the educational implications that follow from a

dual process position (see also Sinatra, 1990). In agreement with

Carlisle and Felbinger (1991), our findings indicate that assessing

both listening and reading comprehension levels with different

types of text would provide a more detailed picture of students’

capabilities and instructional needs. The extent to which listen-

ing level reflects reading potential (Sticht & James, 1984) is lim-

ited and dependent on age and the materials used. As Stanovich

(1991) and Kertoy and Goetz (1995) have pointed out, the diag-

nostic value of any discrepancies between listening and reading is

high. Such discrepancies, however, are more than a simple index of

whether a student is experiencing decoding difficulties as opposed

to more general comprehension problems. They further provide

indications of the ability to process effectively a variety of texts in

different contexts and modalities. Our findings also suggest that

language competence, as a construct, is far too complex to be cap-

tured by a single score obtained in a single assessment context.

Therefore, listening comprehension levels are better thought of

as reflecting the ability to process a particular type of text when

presented orally. The efficiency with which the same type of text

is processed through reading needs to be further established after

decoding mastery.

Our findings also suggest that listening comprehension abil-

ity, just as reading comprehension, is amenable to instruction and

practice (see also Carlisle & Felbinger, 1991). The assumption that

listening ability develops solely as a function of natural language

exposure cannot be taken for granted (Sticht & James, 1984).

Students’ listening ability improves as a function of instructional

practices that facilitate the processing of orally-presented text.

Moreover, our findings suggest that reading instruction needs to

continue beyond decoding mastery to familiarize students with the

content and the structure of different text types (i.e., Horrowitz &

Samuels, 1987) and to provide the context for developing more

76 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

effective comprehension strategies. This appears to be most nec-

essary with expository text (Duke, 2000) that may present greater

difficulty in terms of both content and structure and that eventually

becomes more important for learning purposes. It might be the

case that the ability to comprehend narrative structures precedes

the ability to comprehend expository structures (see also Englert

& Hiebert, 1984). The extent to which this is entirely due to sim-

ple variability in frequency of exposure remains to be seen. On

the basis of our findings, however, and given the research on early

literacy acquisition (Feitelson et al., 1993; Senechal et al., 1998)

we would agree with Duke (2000) that early exposure to oral ex-

pository texts would contribute to the ability to comprehend them

both through listening and reading.

References

Akinnaso, N. F. (1985). On the similarities between spoken and written language.

Language and Speech,28, 323–359.

Biber, D. (1982). On the investigation of spoken/written differences. Studia Lin-

guistica,40, 1–21.

Burns, P. C., & Roe, B. D. (1993). Burns/Roe informal reading inventory: Preprimer to

twelfth grade (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Carlisle, J. F., & Felbinger, L. (1991). Profiles in listening and reading compre-

hension. Journal of Educational Research,84, 345–354.

Curtis, M. E. (1980). Development of components of reading skill. Journal of

Educational Psychology,72, 656–669.

Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture. (1993). National Curriculum—Middle

Education. Nicosia, Cyprus: Government of Cyprus.

Cypriot Ministry of Education and Culture. (1996). National Curriculum—

Elementary Education. Nicosia, Cyprus: Government of Cyprus.

Danks, J. (1980). Comprehension in listening and reading: Same or different? In

J. Danks & K. Pezdek (Eds.), Reading and understanding (pp. 1–39). Newark,

DE: International Reading Association.

Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in

first grade. Reading Research Quarterly,35, 202–224.

Eller, R. G., Pappas, C. C., & Brown, E. (1988). The lexical development of kinder-

garteners: Learning from written context. Journal of Reading Behavior,20, 5–24.

Englert, C. S., & Hiebert, E. H. (1984). Children’s developing awareness of text

structures in expository materials. Journal of Educational Psychology,76, 65–74.

Feitelson, D., Goldstein, Z., Iraqi, J., & Share, D. (1993). Effects of listening to

story reading on aspects of literacy acquisition in a diglossic situation. Reading

Research Quarterly,28, 71–79.

Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. (2000). Structural aspects of constructing mean-

ing from text. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.),

Handbook of reading research (Vol. III, pp. 311–335). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 77

Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during

narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review,101, 371–395.

Graesser, A. C., Swamer, S. S., Baggett, W. B., & Sell, M. A. (1996). New models

of deep comprehension. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of

understanding text (pp. 1–32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Guthrie, J. T., & Tyler, S. J. (1976). Psycholinguistic processing in reading and

listening among good and poor readers. Journal of Reading Behavior,8, 415–

426.

Hendrick, W. B., & Cunningham, J. W. (1995). The relationship between wide

reading and listening comprehension of written language. Journal of Reading

Behavior,27, 425–438.

Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and

Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal,2, 127–160.

Horrowitz, R., & Samuels, S. J. (1987). Comprehending oral and written language:

Critical contrasts for literacy and schooling. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels

(Eds.), Comprehending oral and written language (pp. 1–52). San Diego, CA:

Academic Press.

Johns, J. L. (1994). Basic reading inventory: Pre-primer through grade eight (6th ed.).

Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.

Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: A

simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology,21,

85–97.

Kertoy, M. K., & Goetz, K. M. (1995). The relationship between listening perfor-

mance on the sentence verification technique and other measures of listening

comprehension. Contemporary Educational Psychology,20, 320–339.

Kintsch, W., & Kozminski, E. (1977). Summarizing stories after reading and lis-

tening. Journal of Educational Psychology,69, 491–499.

Levin, I., Levy-Schiff, R., Appelbaum-Peled, T., Katz, I., Komar, M., & Meiran, N.

(1997). Antecedents and consequences of maternal involvement in children’s

homework: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,

18, 207–227.

McNamara, T. P., Miller, D. I., & Bransford, J. D. (1991). Mental models and

reading comprehension. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson

(Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. II, pp. 490–511). New York: Longman.

Neuman, S. B. (1996). Children engaging in storybook reading: The influence of

access to print resources, opportunity, and parental interaction. Early Childhood

Research Quarterly,11, 495–513.

Pearson, P. D., & Fielding, L. (1982). Listening comprehension. Language Arts,

59, 617–629.

Pearson, P. D., & Johnson, D. D. (1978). Teaching reading comprehension. New York:

Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Perfetti, C. A. (1987). Language, speech, and print: Some asymmetries in

the acquisition of literacy. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Compre-

hending oral and written language (pp. 355–369). San Diego, CA: Academic

Press.

Royer, J., M., Kulhavy, R. W., Lee, J. B., & Peterson, S. E. (1986). The sentence

verification technique as a measure of listening and reading comprehension.

Educational and Psychological Research,6, 299–314.

78 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

Royer, J. M., Sinatra, G. M., & Schumer, H. (1990). Patterns of individual differ-

ences in the development of listening and reading comprehension. Contempo-

rary Educational Psychology,15, 183–196.

Samuels, S. J. (1987). Factors that influence listening and reading com-

prehension. In R. Horowitz & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Comprehending

oral and written language (pp. 295–325). San Diego, CA: Academic

Press.

Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential

effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written

language. Reading Research Quarterly,33, 96–116.

Sinatra, G. M. (1990). Convergence of listening and reading processing. Reading

Research Quarterly,25, 115–130.

Singer, M., Harkness, D., & Stewart, S. T. (1997). Constructing inferences in

expository text comprehension. Discourse Processes,24, 199–228.

Smiley, S. S., Oakley, D. D., Worthen, D., Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1977).

Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers

as a function of written vs. oral presentation. Journal of Educational Psychology,

69, 381–387.

Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool

years. Harvard Educational Review,53, 165–189.

Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Discrepancy definitions of reading disability: Has intelli-

gence testing led us astray? Reading Research Quarterly,26, 7–29.

Sticht, T. G., Beck, L. J., Hauke, R. N., Kleinman, G. M., & James, J. H. (1974).

Auding and reading: A developmental model. Alexandria, VA: Human resources

Research Organization.

Sticht, T. G., & James, J. H. (1984). Listening and reading. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr,

M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255–292).

New York: Longman.

Tannen, D. (1985). Relative focus on involvement in oral and written discourse. In

D. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.). Literacy, language, and learning:

The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 124–147). Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

van den Broek, P. (1989). The effects of causal structure on the comprehension

of narratives: Implications for education. Reading Psychology: An International

Quarterly,10, 19–44.

Wells, G. (1986). The meaning makers: Children learning language and using language

to learn. Portsmouth, NH: Heineman.

Appendix

References of Experimental Texts

1. Atmatzidou, M. (1999). Oi aioroumenoi amforeis sti spilia tis

Pafou [The floating amphoras in the Pafos cave]. National ge-

ographic, Greek Edition,2, 22.

The Relationship Between Listening and Reading 79

2. Bach, R. (2000). Psevdesthiseis, oi peripeteies enos distaktikou mes-

sia [Illusions, the adventures of a reluctant messiah], O.

Avramidis, Trans. Athens, Greece: Dioptra.

3. Bartzis, Y. D. (1992). Damokleios spathi [Damocles’ sword].

In Y. D. Bartzis (Ed.), 66 fraseis dyomisi chiliadon chronon [66

phrases two and a half thousand years’ old] (pp. 159–161).

Athens, Greece: Kastanioti.

4. Bartzis, Y. D. (1992). I skia tou onou [The shadow of the don-

key]. In Y. D. Bartzis (Ed.), 66 fraseis dyomisi chiliadon chronon

[66 phrases two and a half thousand years’ old] (pp. 179–180).

Athens, Greece: Kastanioti.

5. Benekou, I. (1999). Enas rinokeros apo to parelthon [A

rhinoceros from the past]. Gaiorama-Experiment,34,4.

6. Chilia chronia efevreseis [One thaousand years of inventions].

(1999, November 21). Erevnites: Paidiko Evdomadiaio Periodiko

[Explorers: Children’s Weekly Magazine], 43, 8–9.

7. De Saint-Exypery, A. (1984). O mikros prigipas [The little

prince], V. Dalakoura, Trans. Athens, Greece: Nefeli.

8. Ekei pou teleionei to vouno [Where the mountain ends].

(1999, March 3). Erevnites: Paidiko Evdomadiaio Periodiko [Ex-

plorers: Children’s Weekly Magazine], 5, 5.

9. Frangia, M. (1999). O Xerxis o xifias [Xerxis, the swordfish].

In M. Frangia (Ed.), To alfavitari tis fisis [Nature’s alphabet]

(pp. 46–48). Athens, Greece: Ellenika Grammata.

10. Frangia, M. (1999). O yfalos [The reef]. In M. Frangia (Ed.),

To alfavitari tis fisis [Nature’s alphabet] (pp. 63–65). Athens,

Greece: Ellenika Grammata.

11. I erimos [The desert] (2002). In M. Bousnakis (Ed.), Egkyk-

lopaideia 2002 [Encyclopaedia 2002] (Vol. 6, p. 385). Nicosia,

Cyprus: Bousnakis.

12. Liverdos, N. (1990). Ta alla taxidia [The other travels]. In

Cypriot Children’s Book Association (Ed.), Paidiki philologia

[Children’s literature] (pp. 148–149). Nicosia, Cyprus: Cypriot

Children’s Book Association.

13. Mesogeiaki fokia [Mediterranean seal] (1999, March 28).

Erevnites: Paidiko Evdomadiaio Periodiko [Explorers: Children’s

Weekly Magazine], 9, 4–5.

14. Paidiki ergasia [Child labor] (1999, May 2). Erevnites: Paidiko

Evdomadiaio Periodiko [Explorers: Children’s Weekly Maga-

zine], 14, 8–9.

80 I.-A. N. Diakidoy et al.

15. To pefko me to retsini pou kolla [The pine with the sticky

resin] (1998). In F. Sakade (Ed.) & G. Tsoulia (Trans.),

Yaponezika paramithia [Japanese folktales] (pp. 55–58). Athens,

Greece: Kedros.

16. To vrefokomeio ton astron [Star nursery] (1999, February, 21).

Erevnites: Paidiko Evdomadiaio Periodiko [Explorers: Children’s

Weekly Magazine], 4, 8–9.